Presents evidence delineating just how much nature shapes our emotional
reactions. Studies on Americans' expressions of anger; Studies on the
Minangkabau, a matrilineal, Moslem agrarian culture; Physiological
changes; Physiological responses to emotions; Common ties of humanity.
In the longest-running debate on human behavior, nature and
nurture have been duking it out for over a century, with nature getting
an awful lot of decisions in the past decade or two. Now comes evidence
delineating just how much nature shapes our emotional reactions.
When Americans create the expressions associated with anger and fear, the autonomic nervous system swings into gear and puts the body on alert, raising heart rate and altering skin temperature. To determine whether these changes are specific to Americans, and thus learned, or are part of a common inheritance, Robert Levenson, Ph.D., of Berkeley, and Paul Ekman, Ph.D., of San Francisco, headed off to West Sumatra. There they looked at people as different from us as you can get: the Minangkabau, a matrilineal, Moslem, agrarian culture that discourages displays of negative emotion.
Yet, when the Minangkabau were taught facial muscle contraction in order to mimic angry or fearful expressions, they registered the same physiologic changes - though they didn't feel the same way. No matter how different we seem, deep down we're all alike, observes the team in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 62, No. 6).
When Americans create the expressions associated with anger and fear, the autonomic nervous system swings into gear and puts the body on alert, raising heart rate and altering skin temperature. To determine whether these changes are specific to Americans, and thus learned, or are part of a common inheritance, Robert Levenson, Ph.D., of Berkeley, and Paul Ekman, Ph.D., of San Francisco, headed off to West Sumatra. There they looked at people as different from us as you can get: the Minangkabau, a matrilineal, Moslem, agrarian culture that discourages displays of negative emotion.
Yet, when the Minangkabau were taught facial muscle contraction in order to mimic angry or fearful expressions, they registered the same physiologic changes - though they didn't feel the same way. No matter how different we seem, deep down we're all alike, observes the team in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 62, No. 6).
But if biological events turn out to be the same, subjective
emotional experience is altogether different. "In our culture, we focus
on the physiological sensations that happen when we feel emotions. This
is in fact one of the most important aspects of emotion for us," reports
Levenson. Ask an American what anger is and he'll tell you what he
physically experiences when he is angry. But the Minangkabau didn't feel
any emotions when they made the negative facial expressions.
"In
their culture, the people are more entwined. Emotions define their
relationships, not bodily sensations," explains Levenson. To them, anger
is when a friend is mad at you, not how your body responds.
"Physiological
responses to emotions are hard-wired into us; they're common for all
people," says Levenson. "But what we do with that information is
culturally variable."
published on January 01, 199
Love,
Abby